Jump to content

User:JMRose00/sandbox

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Measures of implicit self-esteem

[edit]

Implicit self-esteem is assessed using indirect measures of cognitive processing. These include the Name Letter Task[1] and the Implicit Association Test.[2] Such indirect measures are designed to reduce awareness of, or control of, the process of assessment. When used to assess implicit self-esteem, they feature stimuli designed to represent the self, such as personal pronouns (e.g., "I") or letters in one's name.

Name–letter effect

[edit]

The name–letter effect is one of the widest used measures of implicit self-esteem. It represents the idea that an individual prefers the letters belonging to their own name and will select these above other letters in choice tasks. It seemingly occurs subconsciously,[3] with the mere-exposure effect ruled out as a possible explanation.[4]

This effect has been found in a vast range of studies. In one such scenario, participants were given a list of letters, one of which contained letters from their own name and the other of which contained other letters, and asked them to circle the preferred letter. This study found that, even when accounting for all other variables, letters belonging to the participants' own names were preferred.[5]

Similar results have been found in cross-cultural studies, using different alphabets.[6]

It is important to note the difference between the name–letter effect and 'implicit egotism',[7] the latter being attributed to the way people gravitate towards places, people and situations that reflect themselves, including perhaps similarities with their own name. Indeed, research into the topic has shown similarities between people's names and their future careers; for example, the names Dennis and Denise are overepresented among dentists.[8]

Implicit Association Test

[edit]

The Implicit Association Test is an experimental method used within psychology to attempt to tap in to a person's automatic, or subconscious association between a concept and an attribute.[9] It has been widely used in an attempt to uncover a person's subconscious prejudices against certain members of society, such as those who are overweight, as well as other implicit stereotypes and associations. The test was formatted in order to measure self-esteem.[2] Participants are asked to make rapid responses, co-classifying themselves ("the self") and positive attributes, as well as negative attributes. The speed, or ease of these associations made is said to show a subconscious, or implicit preference for one attribute over another, with regards to the self.

  1. ^ Koole, Sander L. (2003). "On the Nature of Implicit Self-Esteem: The Case of the Name Letter Effect". In Steven Spencer, Steven Fein, Mark P. Zanna and James M. Olson (ed.). Motivated social perception. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 93–116. ISBN 0-8058-4036-2. Retrieved 25 April 2009. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: editors list (link)
  2. ^ a b Greenwald, Anthony G.; Farnham, Shelly D. (December 2000). "Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 79 (6): 1022–38. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022. PMID 11138752. Retrieved 25 April 2009.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  3. ^ Koole, S. L., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2001). What's in a name: Implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,669-685.
  4. ^ Jones, J. T., Pelham, B. W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Hetts, J. J. (2002). Name–letter preferences are not merely mere exposure: Implicit egotism as self-regulation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 170-177.
  5. ^ Nuttin, J. M. (1985). Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: The name letter effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15(3), 353-361.
  6. ^ Hoorens, V., Nuttin, J. M., Herman, I. E., & Pavakanun, U. (1990). Mastery pleasure versus mere ownership: A quasi-experimental cross-cultural and cross alphabetical test of the name letter effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20(3), 181-205.
  7. ^ Pelham, B.W., Carvallo, M., & Jones, J.T. (2005). Implicit egoism. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(2), 106-110.
  8. ^ Pelham, B.W., Mirenberg, M.C., & Jones, J.T. (2002) Why Susie Sells Seashells by the Seashore: Implicit Egotism and Major Life Decisions, Attitudes and Social Cognition
  9. ^ Cite error: The named reference social cognition was invoked but never defined (see the help page).